Search the Site

The Economics of Happiness, Part 3: Historical Evidence

Yesterday I noted that there is powerful evidence from the recent Gallup World Poll that rich countries are happier than poor countries. Today, I want to show you how this fact remained hidden in the data for several decades. (And I don’t mean to suggest that we are the first to discover this, but rather that those who noted this were somewhat outside the mainstream.)

In our recent paper, Betsey Stevenson and I went back and re-analyzed all of the early international surveys of happiness, and we now have data from 1946 to 2006.

The figure below shows the results from five early international comparisons of happiness and G.D.P.

The G.D.P. data on the x-axis are self-explanatory. The happiness data on the y-axis are also easy to understand — although the units we use for analyzing them may be less so.

The difficulty we face is that some surveys ask about well-being on a 0 to 10 scale, some ask if you are “very happy, fairly happy, or not very happy”, and others ask about happiness on a 4, 5, and sometimes even 7-point scale.

These differences make it difficult to make comparisons across surveys. We estimate average happiness scores by country by running an ordered probit regression of happiness on country fixed effects, and this is what you see plotted above.

Yes, there is too much jargon in that sentence, so let me try to give an intuitive explanation:

Essentially, this technique assigns each person a happiness “z-score,” by taking their response, dividing it by the standard deviation, and subtracting the mean. We also need to sort out the extent to which “very happy” differs from “fairly happy” or “not very happy,” and the ordered probit does this by analyzing the proportion of the population giving each response.

If, say, only a tiny fraction of people ever respond that they are “not very happy”, then we could infer that this is a pretty bad state to be in. All told, the coefficients can be interpreted as average levels of happiness, measured in units that correspond to the pooled within-country standard deviation of happiness.

If this seems overly complex, the main point is that we try to make these estimates comparable.

To the eye, this figure seems not to show much of a relationship between income and happiness, and early observers simply noted that most of the data was inside the shaded band. But looking more closely, in three of the five cases we get statistically significant effects, and in no case can we reject the hypothesis that the happiness-G.D.P. gradient is 0.3. This is pretty important, as we shall see below.

Next we show the data from succeeding waves of the World Values Survey, which began in the early 1980’s.

INSERT DESCRIPTION

While there is still some doubt about the link between well-being and G.D.P., in the early 1980’s (as more data accumulated) it became increasingly clear that there is a close link between the two. Indeed, by the latest wave of the survey, we see a wellbeing-income gradient of 0.35, and a correlation of 0.7.

Here are some more supportive data from the 2002 Pew Global Attitudes Survey, covering 44 countries:

INSERT DESCRIPTION

Finally, we get to the 2006 Gallup World Poll:

INSERT DESCRIPTION

Yes, this is the same graph I showed yesterday, but this time I’ve put the y-axis in comparable units to the other surveys. And the wellbeing-income gradient is about 0.4.

The key point is that across all of these surveys, the estimated wellbeing-income gradient is around 0.2 to 0.4. Moreover, this holds across each of the many datasets that have accumulated over the past sixty years. In the early years, this relationship was not always statistically significant, but this did not mean that there was no such relationship.

Three more observations about what we can learn from this history:

1) The income-well-being relationship has appeared just about as strongly in surveys probing happiness as in surveys asking about life satisfaction. (There are exceptions.)

2) One interpretation of the 2006 Gallup data is that it is still all about relative income comparisons: In today’s global village, folks in Jamaica may be comparing their lot in life to the greater prosperity they observe when watching U.S. television shows. Countering this, it looks, to my eye, as though the income-happiness link appears about as strong in countries that are truly plugged in to the global village, as those that are less engaged.

3) Moreover, the relationship between income and happiness is about as strong today as it was in the very first surveys, which were taken sixty years ago, when the world was less integrated.

Tomorrow I’ll show how comparisons of rich and poor people also yield a very similar wellbeing-income gradient.


Comments